Two Key Studies of Culture & Prosocial Behaviour (DP IB Psychology)

Revision Note

Claire Neeson

Last updated

Key study one: Levine et al. (2001)

Aim: To investigate prosocial behaviour using the cultural dimension of individualism/collectivism

Procedure:

  • 23 large major cities across the world were selected in which to carry out this large-scale field experiment

  • The cities chosen were classified as coming from either individualistic e.g. USA or collectivist cultures e.g. Malaysia

  • The researchers used psychologists and students from all over the world to help them collect the data

  • The sample consisted of people who were there at the time the behaviour was sampled from the 23 countries selected

  • Thus, the researchers used opportunity sampling

  • The researchers (all male) set up three different scenarios in a variety of outdoor settings involving the following action:

    • A pedestrian drops a pen in the street without noticing

    • A pedestrian wearing a leg brace drops some magazines

    • A blind pedestrian with a cane waits at a traffic light for assistance crossing the street

  • The researchers then observed the rate and degree of help given in each of the above scenarios

Results:

  • Collectivist cultures yielded higher scores for helping behaviour

    • Rio de Janeiro scored the highest at 93% overall followed by San Jose in Costa Rica, Lilongwe in Mali, Calcutta in India

  • Rates and degree of helping tended to be high in countries with low economic productivity

  • One surprising finding was that Vienna ranked fifth as the most helpful city (an individualistic culture and a wealthy city)

  • Another unexpected result was that Kuala Lumpur (the capital of Malaysia, a collectivist culture) ranked lowest out of all the 23 countries in terms of prosocial behaviour (40%)

  • One of the related findings was that there was a positive correlation between slow pace of life (measured by walking speed) and pro-social behaviour 

Conclusion:

  • Cross-cultural differences are apparent in prosocial behaviour

  • Cross-cultural differences in prosocial behaviour are not definitive and may not be the product of culture alone

Evaluation of Levine et al. (2001)

Strengths

  • The huge scale of the research involving 23 countries and hundreds of participants means that the results are generalisable across a range of cultures

    • Generalisability is based on how representative the sample and the measured behaviour is

    • External validity (which encompasses generalisability and ecological validity) is therefore high in this study (

  • The large data set of quantitative findings means that the results are robust

    • quantitative data in large numbers can withstand statistical analysis

    • Statistical analysis is important because it can determine whether a specific variable has an effect on behaviour (e.g. the effect of culture on prosocial behaviour)

 Limitations

  • Replicating this study would be extremely difficult

    • Any replication would necessarily involve a different set of participants

    • Replicating behavioural variables (e.g. dropping newspapers) cannot be replicated exactly and would likely show inconsistency across time

    • This was an ambitious, expensive study which also makes it difficult to replicate

    • Not being able to replicate the study means that its reliability cannot be tested

  • Individual differences may have confounded the results, for example:

    • some people are naturally more helpful than others, regardless of culture

    • external factors such as the weather and time of day may have affected the rate and degree of helping

    • some participants may have been aware that they were being observed and so may have helped more/less than they usually would (the observer effect)

Key study two: Aknin et al. (2013)

Aim: To investigate the idea that human beings around the world derive emotional benefits from donating to charity (prosocial spending)

Participants:

  • 234,917 participants from 136 countries (1,321 per country)

  • The sample was drawn from a representative range of cities, towns and rural areas i.e. both individualistic and collectivist cultures

  • The sample represented 95% of the adult population of the whole world at the time of the study’s publication

  • 51% of the participants were female; 49% were male, with a total mean age of 38 years

Procedure:

  • A correlational analysis of data was conducted based on interviews conducted with each participant

  • A 30-minute phone interview was conducted with participants from more affluent countries

  • A one-hour face-to-face interview was conducted with participants from less affluent poorer countries

  • Participants were asked to give a yes/no response as to whether they had donated money to a charity in the past month

    • The yes/no response was followed up with a question pertaining to their subjective well-being, for example: 

      • rate how satisfied you are with your life as a whole on a 10-point scale (0, dissatisfied, to 10, satisfied). 

Results:

  • Prosocial spending was positively correlated with higher reported feelings of subjective well-being around the world

  • The above finding held true whether the country was individualistic or collectivist, rich or poor

Conclusion: Spending money on prosocial causes may be linked to feelings of well-being which may be universal across cultures

Evaluation of Aknin et al. (2013)

Strengths

  • The sample obtained for this study is almost unique in psychological research in terms of its representative nature: 

    • it can be generalised to almost any adult from anywhere in the world

  • The findings have good application, for example:

    • charities can use the idea that donating gives people a ‘buzz’ i.e. donating for selfish reasons (which, nonetheless, benefit the charity)

    • on a smaller scale, schools and other institutions such as hospitals and prisons could use the findings to promote helpfulness as part of their well-being and mental health strategies

 Limitations

  • Donating money to charity is held in high regard in all societies so it is possible that some participants may have exaggerated their prosocial spending

    • The above observation means that social desirability bias may have influenced the participants’ responses

    • Social desirability bias lowers the validity of a study’s findings

  • Although the link between prosocial spending and subjective well-being was strong, there was still variability between countries

Variability in results means that the researchers were not able to state definitively that prosocial spending is a universal behaviour

Worked Example

ERQ (EXTENDED RESPONSE QUESTION) 22 MARKS

The question is, ‘Discuss key factors that influence prosocial behaviour across cultures’ [22]

This question is asking you to  provide a considered and balanced review of key factors of prosocial behaviour across cultures that includes a range of arguments, viewpoints and factors. Your conclusions should be presented clearly and supported by relevant studies. Here are two paragraphs for guidance:

One key factor in cross-cultural prosocial behaviour is that of low economic productivity. The idea that a city’s ‘personality’ affects individual behaviour is known as the simpatico hypothesis: people in communities where social obligations take priority over individual achievements tend to be less economically productive, but show more willingness to assist others. This idea was explored in a large-scale field study by Levine et al (2001). 23 large major cities across the world comprised the sample, using naïve participants. The researchers (all male) set up three different scenarios in a variety of outdoor settings involving a pen being dropped, a disabled person dropping magazines and a blind person waiting at traffic lights. The researchers observed the rate and degree of help given in each situation.

Levine et al’s results were as expected and also not as expected to some extent: the most helpful cities were also those in which collectivism was more of an influence than individualism (Rio de Janeiro was the highest at 93% overall, San Jose in Costa Rica, Lilongwe in Mali, Calcutta in India). Helping rates tended to be high in countries with low economic productivity with low purchasing power for each citizen. However, not all results obtained reflect this tendency: the one surprising finding here was that Vienna ranked fifth as the most helpful city; Vienna is a wealthy European capital which does not fulfil the criteria for collectivism as the other cities in the top five do. Another unexpected result was that Kuala Lumpur (the capital of Malaysia) ranked lowest out of all the 23 countries in terms of prosocial behaviour (40%). One of the related findings was that there was a positive correlation between slow pace of life (measured by walking speed) and pro-social behaviour. 

You've read 0 of your 10 free revision notes

Unlock more, it's free!

Join the 100,000+ Students that ❤️ Save My Exams

the (exam) results speak for themselves:

Did this page help you?

Claire Neeson

Author: Claire Neeson

Expertise: Psychology Content Creator

Claire has been teaching for 34 years, in the UK and overseas. She has taught GCSE, A-level and IB Psychology which has been a lot of fun and extremely exhausting! Claire is now a freelance Psychology teacher and content creator, producing textbooks, revision notes and (hopefully) exciting and interactive teaching materials for use in the classroom and for exam prep. Her passion (apart from Psychology of course) is roller skating and when she is not working (or watching 'Coronation Street') she can be found busting some impressive moves on her local roller rink.