Two Key Studies of Bystanderism (DP IB Psychology)
Revision Note
Key study one: Latane & Darley (1968)
Aim:
To investigate the the bystander effect
To investigate diffusion of responsibility (DOR) as the key explanation for bystanderism
Participants:
72 students (59 female, 13 male) on an introductory psychology course from New York University
The researchers had contacted the participants to ask for their participation but the details of the research were not revealed to the participants in advance
Procedure:
Each participant was placed alone at a table with headphones and a microphone on it
Each participant was then told that they would be taking part in a discussion of problems that college students faced:
due to the potentially sensitive nature of the topic they would be alone in the room
they would be using the microphone and headphones to conduct the discussion
The participants were in the middle of listening to another person (a confederate) speaking when it became clear that the person on the other end of the intercom was having a seizure
The independent variable was designed as follows:
The ‘alone’ condition: the participant believed that they alone were listening to the seizure
The 3-person group: the participant believed that one other person was listening in
The 6-person group: the participant believed that four other people were listening in
The dependent variable was measured as the time it took the participant to seek assistance to help the person who appeared to be having a seizure
Results:
In the ‘alone’ condition 85% of the participants reported the seizure
In the ‘3 person group’ 62% of the participants reported the seizure
In the ‘6 person group’ 31% of the participants reported the seizure
Conclusion:
DOR may to some extent explain the theory of the unresponsive bystander:
less help is likely to be given in an emergency if people believe that there are others present who can offer help
less help may be given due to factors such as:
thinking that someone else will help (more likely if in a larger group)
not feeling competent to offer help (again, this is more likely in a larger group, particularly if the emergency requires medical intervention)
Evaluation of Latané & Darley (1968)
Strengths
This is a well-designed study with three distinct conditions of the independent variable
group size did seem to have an influence as to the speed and rate of helping, thus it supports the DOR hypothesis
The researchers reported that many of the participants were genuinely distressed at the sound of someone appearing to have a seizure
This finding gives the study good external validity
Limitations
It is possible that some of the participants may have guessed the aim of the study, which would lower the validity of the findings
There are ethical considerations to the procedure which mean that it would not be replicable today:
the participants were deceived as to the true nature of the seizures
as the participants were deceived they could not give fully informed consent
the participants experienced distress and shock when listening to the faked seizures which caused them harm
Key study two: Piliviavin et al. (1969)
Aim
To conduct a field experiment to investigate the effect of several different variables on the bystander effect:
how quickly and how frequently help is given
the type of victim (drunk or ill)
the ethnicity/race of victim (Black or White) i.e. the Just World Hypothesis (JWH)
DOR in specific conditions
the cost-benefit analysis
Participants:
An opportunity sample of 4,450 passengers (55% white; 45% black)
The passengers were those who used the New York subway between Harlem and The Bronx during the hours of 11am until 3pm
The procedure was repeated on the same line over the course of several months
The journey lasted 7.5 minutes without any stops.
Procedure:
4 confederates were used per trial:
2 females as observers
1 White male aged 24 – 29 to model helping behaviour
1 male victim aged 26 – 35 (either White or Black)
The victims were either ‘drunk’ (he smelled of alcohol) or ‘ill’ (he held a cane)
The victims were dressed identically to each other
A total of 103 trials was conducted by alternating teams of researchers over the total course of the research’s duration
The female confederates took seats and kept notes as unobtrusively as possible
The male victim and male model stood near a pole in the centre of the train
After passing the first station (approximately 70 seconds) the victim collapsed
In the “no help” condition, the model did nothing until the train slowed to a stop, and then helped the victim to his feet
In the “helping” condition, the model came to the victim’s assistance
There were four different helping conditions used in both “drunk” and “cane” situations:
Critical area – early: The model stood in the critical area (the carriage in which the victim stood) and waited until after the train passed the fourth station, and then helped the victim (approximately 70 seconds after collapse)
Critical area – late: The model stood in the critical area and waited until after the train passed the sixth station before helping the victim (approximately 150 seconds after collapse)
Adjacent area – early: The model stood in the adjoining carriage, adjacent to the critical area and waited until after the train passed the fourth station, and then helped the victim
Adjacent area – late: The model stood in the adjacent area and waited until after the sixth station before helping
Results:
The table shows the help given dependent on condition (victim type and ethnicity/race):
Condition - victim and ethnicity | Help given |
Ill (with cane) | 95% (62 out of 65 trials) |
Drunk (smelling of alcohol) | 50% (19 out of 38 trials) |
White (no model help) - Ill | 100% |
White (no model help) – Drunk | 100% |
Black (no model help) - Ill | 100% |
Black (no model help) - Drunk | 73% |
Spontaneous help (before 70 seconds had passed) in the model trials was more likely in the ‘ill’ condition
The victims received help for a total of 81/103 trials, and in 60% of these cases the help was received from more than one person
Once the first person had moved to help, two or three others often followed quite quickly
Men were more likely to help than women:
some of the women’s comments were along the lines of:
‘It’s a man’s job to help’
‘I wouldn’t know what to do’
Same-race helping was noted in the Black/drunk condition more than in other conditions
Conclusion:
DOR was not supported by the results of this study
in fact the larger the group the more help was given
Help may have prompted by the need to reduce feelings of anxiety and guilt:
there was ‘no escape’ possible from the victim as the passengers were ‘trapped’ in a subway carriage
a cost-benefit analysis of the situation would have resulted in a higher cost for not helping than for helping
Type of victim is a factor in whether help is given, with the ‘ill’ victim receiving more help than the ‘drunk’ victim
Evaluation of Piliavin et al. (1969)
Strengths
This was a highly organised, well-designed study that collected both quantitative and qualitative data:
quantitative data (speed and frequency of helping) derived from a large sample size is high in reliability
qualitative data (from the observations) is high in ecological validity due to the natural setting and unforced behaviour of the participants
The finding that DOR may not occur outside of lab settings is hugely interesting and casts the topic of bystanderism in a new light.
Limitations
The study might have been affected by a range of extraneous variables that were impossible to control:
mood of each passenger on the day (good mood is likely to increase helping behaviour)
some of the participants experiencing the procedure more than once if they regularly travelled on that route (which would create demand characteristics)
participants in the carriage may have obscured the view of the emergency, meaning that some data may have been lost or misinterpreted by the observers
The ethics of the study were compromised due to
lack of informed consent
deception of participants
no right to withdraw
possible psychological harm and distress
no debriefing
Worked Example
ERQ (EXTENDED RESPONSE QUESTION) 22 MARKS
The question is, ‘Evaluate research into bystanderism.’ [22]’
This essay is asking you to assess the strengths and limitations of one or more explanations relating to bystanderism, with the emphasis being placed on the arguments and methods used by the researchers. Here are two paragraph for guidance:
One strength of Piliavin et al. (1969) is the use of a real setting and the length of time in which the procedure was carried out, being repeated the same way for every trial. The study was carried out in the field over several months, taking place in one carriage of a New York subway train, involving a total of 4,450 naïve participants, thus the ecological validity and generalisability were high. The participants would have had no inkling that they were taking part in a study – which raises some ethical concerns but the nature of the research meant that this was unavoidable – so their behaviour could be confidently said to be valid and unforced.
Piliavin et al.’s results demonstrate a reversal in bystander apathy findings: all prior research had shown a strong tendency towards diffusion of responsibility. This is undoubtedly due to the unique nature of Piliavin et al’s methodology: a field experiment carried out in near-lab conditions using a ‘captive’ audience of participants who could not escape from the immediate scene of the emergency. According to the cost-benefit analysis the levels of discomfort experienced by the passengers would have produced too much of a cost – by helping the victim their discomfort (and the cost of not helping) would have evaporated. It is entirely possible that had Piliavin et al conducted this study on the street then their findings would have supported the concept of bystander apathy.
You've read 0 of your 5 free revision notes this week
Sign up now. It’s free!
Did this page help you?