Two Key Studies of Cognitive Biases: Hamilton & Gifford (1976) & Piliavin et al. (1969) (HL IB Psychology)
Revision Note
Written by: Claire Neeson
Reviewed by: Lucy Vinson
Examiner Tip
You can also use Hamilton & Gifford (1976) to answer a question on the Formation of Stereotypes (Sociocultural Approach) and you can use Piliavin et al. (1969) to answer a question on the Bystander Effect (Relationships option in Year 2 of the IB Psychology course)
Key Study: Hamilton & Gifford (1976)
Aim: To investigate illusory correlation as a cognitive bias
Participants: 40 undergraduate students from a university in New York state, USA (20 males; 20 females)
Procedure:
The participants were presented with two hypothetical groups - i.e. these were not real groups consisting of real people with given characteristics. The participants were told that Group A consisted of 26 members and that Group B consisted of 13 members
The participants then read a series of statements which each described a particular behaviour performed by either a member of A or B e.g. John, a member of A, visited a friend in hospital
The behaviours described in the statements were classified as either desirable or undesirable. Both A and B were assigned more positive than negative behaviours at a ratio of 9:4 (positive to negative) and two thirds of the statements overall were attributed to members of A
Thus, members of A were presented as performing more behaviours overall than B and positive behaviours were more frequent from both groups than negative behaviours
The participants were then asked to provide ratings for the following measures:
Given a list of 20 attributes, assign each to either group A or B
Given a particular example of a behaviour, say whether this behaviour was performed by a member of A or B
Estimate how many negative behaviours can be attributed to either A or B
Results: The mean scores showed that participants attributed more desirable social behaviours (6.7) to members of Group A than to members of Group B (6.0); undesirable social behaviours were attributed more to Group B (5.6) than to Group A (4.4)
Conclusion: The results suggest that illusory correlation may be based on group size: the smaller group, B, appears more distinctive than the larger group A so that any undesirable behaviours are linked more often to the minority group, B, than to the majority group A. This has implications in terms of how minority groups are viewed by society
Evaluation of Hamilton & Gifford (1976)
Strengths
The measures (rating scales) used in the study could be cross-referenced by the researchers to check for consistency across them which should ensure both reliability and internal validity
The findings could be used to inform awareness-raising as a means to reduce prejudice and increase tolerance of minority groups
Limitations
The procedure does not fully reflect how people respond in real-life situations where they are exposed to minority groups which reduces ecological validity
The study is rather simplistic in its use of statements about hypothetical people and situations which makes it difficult to draw very meaningful conclusions from the findings
Key terms:
Illusory correlation
Minority
Majority
Key Study: Piliavin et al. (1969)
Aim: To investigate the degree of help given to a victim who appeared to be either drunk or disabled
Participants: A field experiment which used an opportunity sample of 4,450 passengers (55% white; 45% black) using the New York subway between Harlem and The Bronx during the hours of 11am until 3pm over the course of several months. The journey lasted 7.5 minutes without any stops
Procedure:
A staged (fake) procedure which was conducted inside one carriage of the aforementioned Harlem/Bronx subway route. 4 confederates were used: 2 females as observers, 1 white male aged 24 – 29 to model helping behaviour and 1 male victim aged 26 – 35 (either white or black, dressed identically; the ‘drunk’ victim smelled of alcohol and the ‘cane’ victim had a cane to indicate that he was disabled)
103 trials were conducted by alternating teams of researchers over the total course of the research’s duration
The female confederates took seats and kept notes, while the male victim and male model stood near a pole in the centre of the train
After passing the first station (approximately 70 seconds into the journey) the victim collapsed
In the “no help” condition, the model did nothing until the train slowed to a stop, and then helped the victim to his feet
In the “helping” condition, the model came to the victim’s assistance
The collapse occurred in what the researchers referred to as the critical area which was in the immediate vicinity of the victim
There were four different helping conditions used in both “drunk” and “cane” situations:
Critical area early: the model stood in the critical area and waited approximately 70 seconds after the collapse to help
Critical area late: the model stood in the critical area and waited approximately 150 seconds after the collapse to help
Adjacent area early: the model stood a little further way, adjacent to the critical area and waited approximately 70 seconds after the collapse then helped the victim
Adjacent area late: the model stood a little further way, adjacent to the critical area and waited approximately 150 seconds after the collapse then helped the victim
Results: The victim in the ‘cane’ condition received spontaneous help on 95% of the trials (62 out of 65 times) - i.e. there was very little need for the model to help first; people helped the apparently disabled man immediately upon his collapse. The ‘drunk’ condition received help on 50% of the trials (19 out of 38 times)
Conclusion: The results support the Just-World Hypothesis as the victim in the ‘cane’ condition was helped 50% more than the victim who appeared to be drunk. It is possible that
people operate a system of judgement when deciding who to give help to - i.e. does the victim ‘deserve’ help or not?
Evaluation of Piliavin et al. (1969)
Strengths
The study is high in ecological validity due to the use of the natural setting and unartificial behaviour of the naïve participants
The use of two observers should ensure inter-rater reliability
Limitations
The procedure is likely to have been affected by a range of extraneous variables that were impossible to control, for example:
individual differences such as personality and mood
some of the participants experiencing the procedure more than once if they used that route regularly
participants in the carriage obscuring the view of the observers
The ethics of the study are problematic:
no informed consent
deception of participants
possible psychological harm
no right to withdraw
no debriefing
Key terms:
Just-World hypothesis
Field experiment
Confederates
Examiner Tip
With a study such as Piliavin et al. (1969) do ensure that you have learnt all of the parts of the study in as much detail as possible: it is not often that field experiments feature in your IB Psychology course and this particular one offers a wealth of potential critical thinking points.
For example, the study generated both quantitative (in the form of number of helpers) and qualitative data (in the form of passenger’s comments and body language) and used a standardised procedure in a natural setting which is a very difficult task to undertake and one which not many other researchers have managed to match
Last updated:
You've read 0 of your 10 free revision notes
Unlock more, it's free!
Did this page help you?