The Ethological Explanation of Aggression (AQA A Level Psychology)
Revision Note
Written by: Claire Neeson
Reviewed by: Lucy Vinson
The adaptive & ritualistic functions of aggression
Ethology is the study of animal behaviour and, by extension, human behaviour in terms of traits, characteristics and rituals which have adapted to become useful for survival
Aggression is an evolutionary process in that it is designed to minimise conflict and spread resources around the group (whether that be lions, monkeys, sticklebacks or humans)
In the animal kingdom there are specific territorial behaviours which help to establish who is ‘top dog’ (quite literally!) and who must take their place further down the pecking order
Territoriality-linked aggressive behaviour may take the form of stags rutting; male sticklebacks displaying a red underbelly as a warning to other males; a cat arching its back and hissing at other cats to ‘back off’
Aggressive territoriality is a useful survival strategy as it results in one animal claiming rights over a specific territory which means that the rest of the group have to find alternative feeding and breeding areas which spreads the resources around and prevents overpopulation of one area (and possible starvation)
Ritualistic behaviours are designed to show competitors who is ‘boss’ and to deter them from encroaching on the chosen territory
Ritualistic behaviours are characterised in the following ways:
Little harm is actually done to each animal, the threat is implied rather than carried out to its natural conclusion i.e. death, which is ultimately damaging to the species’ survival
Signs of threat may include a lowered head; baring of teeth/fangs; growling/snarling; the display of red in feathers/coat/skin
Signs of surrender include birds turning their head towards a competitor (this is the most vulnerable part of the bird and signals the end to the fight); dogs and wolves assuming a submissive position e.g. lowering themselves down or (according to Lorenz) exposing their throat (again, the most vulnerable part of the animal)
Human behaviour has some similarities with animal aggression e.g. wars are fought for territories; males may ‘posture’ in front of other males to warn them away from a desired female e.g. via lots of shouting, pushing, arms spread wide etc.
Who would have thought that this cute little chap is showing how dangerous he is!
Examiner Tips and Tricks
You should definitely cover examples of animal threat and display in a question on ethological explanations of aggression but don’t forget to make the link to human behaviour (unless the question does not require this).
Innate releasing mechanisms & fixed action patterns
An innate releasing mechanism (IRM) is a response to a specific stimulus: the process is instinctive i.e. it does not have to be learned
IRMs are thought to comprise a neural network in the brain i.e. they are inbuilt biological structures
IRMs may be conspecific (signalled to members of the same species) or allospecific (signalled to animals of different species)
Examples of IRM include:
Male sticklebacks signal aggression via their red underbellies: this triggers an IRM in another male stickleback who may respond by also showing aggression or by moving away from the threat (Tinbergen, 1951)
A dog sees a cat running away, so the dog’s IRM produces an innate response: ‘chase the cat!’
A consequence of an IRM is a fixed action pattern (FAP) e.g. the male stickleback notices a threat so he begins a series of predetermined actions and behaviours that are designed to deter any intruders or competitors; moths fold their wings to limit ultrasonic sounds as predators use these sounds to hunt them
FAPs tend to be: unchanging; universal across the species; stable across the species; inevitable; situation-specific; not affected by learning
Moths fold in their wings to prevent predators from detecting them: an example of a fixed action pattern.
Research which investigates the ethological explanation of aggression
Lorenz (1970) - the ‘founding father’ of ethological research; he first proposed IRMs and conducted various studies on animal behaviour as a guide to human behaviour
Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1967) - neural mechanisms and social patterns of aggression in animals and humans
Cohen et al. (1996) - threats (real or implied) were met with more aggressive responses from males from the southern states of the USA than males from the northern states i.e. there may cultural differences in aggression based on learned social norms
Evaluation of the ethological explanation of aggression
Strengths
There is some validity to this theory from both animal and human studies (e.g. Raine et al. 1997 who found that impulsive murderers respond without thinking/instinctively to stimuli)
Some human aggression may be as a result of genes (and hence, innate and instinctive) as seen in Brunner et al.’s (1993) study on aggression and a defective MAOA gene
Weaknesses
As with any research based on animal behaviour it is extremely difficult to generalise to human beings who are more cognitively and socially sophisticated and nuanced than animals
Human researchers are not best-placed to fully understand animal behaviour; interpretations and inferences only can be drawn from the behaviour of other species
Link to Issues & Debates:
This topic fits into the nature/nurture debate as it claims that some human behaviours may be instinctive i.e. purely the result of evolution and adaptation across generations (the nature side of the debate).
Worked Example
Describe and evaluate the ethological explanation of aggression. Refer to relevant research in your answer. [16]
AO1 = 6 marks, AO3 = 10 marks.
For 13-16 marks knowledge is accurate, well detailed and evaluation is effective. The response is clear, coherent and focused with effective use of terminology.
For 9 – 12 marks knowledge is present but with occasional inaccuracies/omissions and mostly effective evaluation. It is mostly clear and organised with an occasional lack of focus and some use of terminology.
For 5 - 8 marks there is limited knowledge and the main focus of the answer is description. Evaluation is fairly limited with a lack of terminology. Clarity, accuracy and organisation are at times lacking.
For 1 - 4 marks knowledge is very limited with weak discussion (evaluation may be entirely absent). The answer lacks clarity, has many inaccuracies and is poorly organised with sparse or absent terminology.
Possible answer content could include:
AO1: The ethological explanation is based on evolutionary mechanisms for dominance over territory/resources/mates and is essentially adaptive as it establishes hierarchies within the animal world as researched by Pettit (1988)
Specific rituals are a feature of animal aggression as Lorenz observed in his work on conspecific aggression in wolves. Innate releasing mechanisms (IRMs) may trigger fixed action patterns (FAPs) for example the red markings on male sticklebacks (Tinbergen, 1951) or moths folding in their wings when in the presence of a predator
AO3: There is some validity to the idea that aggression is an innate reflex/response both in animals and in humans (Brunner’s 1993 study on the MAOA gene linked to aggression in affected males of one family)
Some aggression may not be innate/biological but may be the result of learning and culture, for example, Cohen’s ‘culture of honour’ (1996) research on differences in aggression depending on where in the USA participants were from
FAPs may, however not be entirely innate but could also be influenced by environment as well i.e. they consist of several different behaviours in a series, varying from one organism to another (Hunt, 1973)
Using animal models to draw conclusions about human behaviour lacks generalisability and may be overly deterministic as it assumes an inevitability about human responses devoid of cognitive mediation i.e. humans are more complex and less instinctive than animals
Last updated:
You've read 0 of your 10 free revision notes
Unlock more, it's free!
Did this page help you?